document2

43
GEK1532 Color Perception Mechanisms and Binocular Vision Seeing the light, Fig. 10.11 Thorsten Wohland Dep. Of Chemistry S8-03-06 Tel.: 6516 1248 E-mail: [email protected]

Upload: phuochiep123

Post on 19-Jun-2021

0 views

Category:

Social Media


0 download

Tags:

DESCRIPTION

erhhregre

TRANSCRIPT

  • GEK1532
    Color Perception Mechanisms and Binocular Vision

    Seeing the light, Fig. 10.11

    Thorsten Wohland

    Dep. Of Chemistry

    S8-03-06

    Tel.: 6516 1248

    E-mail: [email protected]

  • Textbook

    Color vision: Perspective from different disciplines, BackhausLight Vision Color, A. Valberg

  • Please read until next week:

    Saunders and Brakel:

    http://www.bbsonline.org/Preprints/OldArchive/bbs.saunders.html

  • Japanese Bridge over

    Water Lily Pond 1926

    Japanese Bridge over

    Water Lily Pond 1899

    House seen from the rose garden 1924

    House seen from the rose garden 1924

  • Retina independent color anomalies

    With age the lens of humans becomes more and more yellow (same happens with cataracts).

    Your brain adapts to that and you still perceive white as white etc.

    However, when you paint, the colors you use will contain more yellow (Metamers).

  • The organization of the retina

  • Spatial summation

    T.N. Cornsweet, Fig. 2.5

    Illuminate spots on the retina of different size and determine the number of photons needed before the spot can be seen

    1st spot: only few rods on average

    2nd spot: smaller than summation area

    3rd spot: larger than summation area

    Sensitivity constant

    Sensitivity decreases

  • Temporal summation

    Adapted form T.N. Cornsweet, Fig. 2.5

    How many photons have to arrive in a certain time interval so that the eye sees a flash?

    0

    10

    20

    time (ms)

    0

    10

    20

    time (ms)

    0

    10

    20

    time (ms)

  • Lateral Inhibition

    One ganglion cell receives signal from many receptors, excitatory or inhibitory signals.

    One cone/rod can contribute to some ganglion cells excitatory to others inhibitory.

    STL Fig. 7.2

    +

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    +

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    +

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    +

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    +

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

  • Lateral Inhibition

    STL Fig. 7.12

    rest

    excitation

    inhibition

    No difference -> rest

    Strong excitation

    No difference -> rest

  • Lateral Inhibition

    STL Fig. 7.8

  • Spatial frequency and tilt

  • If edge information is missing …

  • Afterimages

    You can have negative and positive afterimages.

    The effect comes from the fact that when a cone/rod is stimulated for a long time it “desensitizes”.

    1) The cones perceiving the black square are not excited, the cones perceiving the white surrounding are excited and desensitize with time.

    2) When looking at the white surface on the right, the desensitized cones are less excited than the rested cones in the middle and thus you see a white square.

  • Negative after images

    rest

    excitation

    Inhibition or desensitization

    STL Fig. 7.12

    Inhibition: If an excited cone, i.e. a cone that has absorbed light suppresses signaling, it is called inhibition. The result is a lower frequency of firing of the ganglion cell.

    Desensitization: After strong excitation a cone can become less sensitive and cannot react again immediately. In this case there could be as well less firing from this cone.

  • Negative after images

    rest

    excitation

    Inhibition or desensitization

    STL Fig. 7.12

    S

    M

    L

    Cones

    Long exposure to white light

    No image

    Long exposure of some cones, image is seen

    The exposed cones are desensitized, give lower signal than surrounding rested cones.

  • Afterimages

    Positive afterimages.

    You can sensitize your retina by closing your eyes and resting your cones (remember when you close eyes a long time and open them you seem to be blinded first).

    When you open your eyes shortly (seconds) and look at some bright object the cones get excited.

    When you close your eyes again the cones will not desensitize and will stay stimulated longer and give you a positive afterimage.

    See the TRY IT on page 194 of STL.

    http://www.michaelbach.de/ot/

  • Now let’s recall what we know about the CIE system and then let’s see whether there are any facts left unexplained.

    Can we perhaps resolve some of these issues with our new knowledge of the retina and its organization?

  • Trichromacy, Tristimulus theory

    l

    Sensitivity

    Take one cone; shine light of constant intensity on the cone; measure the light transmitted; calculate absorption

  • Color mixing

    l

    Sensitivity

    x

    0

    1.0

    x: equal excitation of blue and green cone by 30%, no excitation of red

    If P1, P2, and x have same intensity we have too much red.

    Since P3 excites the red cone 4 times less P2, we can subtract 4 times P3 to get our mixture:

    x = P1 + P2 – 4 P3

    P1: excitation of blue cone by 30%, no excitation of green and red

    P1

    P2: excitation of green cone by 30%, excitation of red by 80%, no excitation of blue

    P2

    P3

    P3: excitation only of red by 20%.

  • Negative values: 3 primaries are not enough to mix all colors

    3 abstract colors are chosen which then can cover all visible colors with positive values.

    These colors do not exist, and some of their mixtures do not give real colors either.

    The normalization, the condition that x+y+z=1 allows us then to depict all colors in one graph, but only at constant intensity.

  • The CIE system

    Complementary colors are connected by a straight line going through white.

    www.adobe.com

  • The CIE system

    Mixtures of colors are easy to find.

    Distance from 486 nm point is three times longer than from 545 nm point.

    Therefore you need a mixture of 486:545 nm of 1:3.

    www.adobe.com

  • The CIE system

    It can be easily found how to construct metamers.

    www.adobe.com

  • Complementarity

    Do all spectral hues have a complementary spectral hue?

    STL, Fig. 9.9

  • Hue discrimination

    STL, Fig. 10.4

  • Remember this?

    www.adobe.com

    Copyright ©2000 Adobe Systems Incorporated. All rights reserved.
    Information is provided "As Is" without warranty of any kind. Users may make a single copy of portions of database for personal use provided that this notice is included on such copy.

  • Facts not explained by Trichromacy
    Color naming

    Experiment done by asking a person to estimate how much blue, yellow, green, and red is contained in a hue represented by a pure wavelength.

    STL, Fig. 10.9

  • Hue cancellation

    STL, Fig. 10.10

  • Opponent processing

    STL: Fig. 7.2

    Can we connect the cones in a fashion, so that the signal at the ganglion cells will correspond to the four opponent colors red, green, blue, and yellow?

  • Possible combinations

    S

    M

    L

    +

    Perceived Brightness

    M

    L

    -

    Red-Green

  • Possible combinations

    Blue - Yellow

    So we have constructed 3 new signals from the original three cones:

    Black – WhiteGreen – RedBlue – Yellow

    … based on 4 colors

    +

    M

    L

    Yellow

    -

    S

  • Four color opponent model

    Seeing the light, Fig. 10.11

  • CIE and the opponent process

    STL, Fig. 10.12

    W: all cones are equally excited, therefore the lines dividing the CIE in r, g, b, y regions must cross there.

    W: all cones are equally excited, therefore the lines dividing the CIE in r, g, b, y regions must cross there.

  • Spatial Processing of Color

  • Double opponency

    One ganglion cell receives signal from many receptors, excitatory or inhibitory signals.

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    +

    The combination of both gives double opponency

    Opponency of location (inside versus outside)

    Opponency of color

  • Double Opponency

    M

    L

    -

    Red - Green

    1. Individual L and M cone signals are pooled by a ganglion cell to give a Red-Green opponent signal

    2. Depending on the position of the cones on your retina the Red-Green opponent signal can work as excitatory (+) some as inhibitory (-) signals.

    M

    L

    -

    Green - Red

    +

    - -

    - -

    - -

    E.g. a red surface

  • Revision: Spatial Processing of Color

    STL, Fig. 10.16

    +

    +

    +

    +

    +

    +

    +

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    -

    +

    +

    +

    +

    +

  • Revision: Spatial Processing of Color

    0

    0

    +

    +

    0

    0

    0

    -

    -

    -

    0

    0

    STL, Fig. 10.16

  • Temporal Processing

    STL, Fig. 10.19

    Benham disk:

    White parts excite all three cones. However, the three cones recover from activation differently. When black falls onto the excited cones, some are still stimulated (e.g. the blue one) while others (red and green) have already recovered. Thus one sees blue.

  • Temporal Processing

    STL, Fig. 10.21

  • S

    M

    L

    Cones

    No excitation, no color perception

    Flash of white light

    All cone excited, white is seen

    Red cone de-excites fast, a blue/green (cyan) color is seen

    Green cone de-excites next. Blue is seen

    Return to resting state afetr blue cone has de-excited as well

    Benham disk, positive afterimages

  • Summary

    Color Perception MechanismsTristimulus TheoryColor naming, hue cancellationOpponent processingSpatial Processing of ColorTemporal Processing of Color